By observing the behavioral responses of mares during the implementation of touch and leading, using two different handling methods, our findings show the improvement in mare’s reception of touch and leading when using TTEAM compared to T. These results are in line with previous body of research showing the effect of TTEAM or similar natural methods on reduced fear, reactivity and physiological stress in young horses
(Polito et al., 2007; Visser et al., 2009).
Touch protocol
A significant difference was found in the touch scores between mares (F = 20.51; η
2 = 0.56; P<0.001). The score ranged from 1.4 to 5 (Fig 3). Scores significantly differed between testing days (F = 4.86; η
2 = 0.19; P<0.001). During the first six days, the score was constant and there was no change in score when the method was changed. From the 8
th to 12
th day there was a gradual increase in scores (Fig 4). Regardless of the method, numerically the highest score (Fig 4) was found on the last day of the method application while the lowest score (Fig 4) on the 2
nd day of the method implication. There was a significantly higher touch score in the TTEAM (4.6±0.9) compared to T (4.1±1.2; Z = -4.45; P<0.001; Fig 4). Generally, a wide spectrum of tactile interactions results in the activation of the central nervous system, by targeting neurocircuitry such as the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex
(Lindgren et al., 2012), which an individual usually perceives as pleasant
(Morrison et al., 2010). In horses, proximity and tactile interaction, for example allogrooming is important for establishing and maintaining social bonds
(Wolter et al., 2018). It translates also to horse-human interaction, as stroking or massage to some extent imitates natural allogrooming and has previously been reported to induce positive behavioral responses
(McBride et al., 2004).
Touch also presents a social signal for safety in human-animal interaction, however, positive human-directed behavioral responses in horses, as a result of touching, have only been observed while touching preferred body parts for grooming, such as croup
(McBride et al., 2004). Similarly, in our study, more positive behaviors towards the experimenter and better reception of touch resulting in higher touch scores, were shown during TTEAM. This may be the result of relaxation caused by the initial massage-like pressure to the neck
(McBride et al., 2004). Also, the influencing factor may be the body part on which the touch was first applied. During TTEAM it was on the whither which is a preferred site for grooming, whereas during T it was around the ears, well known as a non-preferred site for grooming
(McBride et al., 2004).
There was a significant difference in touch duration between mares (F = 11.12; η
2 = 0.50; P<0.001). The duration ranged between 10.9±2.6 s and 28.5±1.1 s (Fig 3). Duration significantly differed between testing days (F = 8.67, η
2 = 0.29; P<0.0001) but was consistent from 1
st up to 6
th day. The biggest difference in duration was between the 6
th day and 7
th day (P<0.001) and then significantly decreased on the 10
th and 11
th day. The touch duration was significantly higher in the TTEAM compared to T (t = -4.85, P<0.0001; Fig 4). In order to observe mare’s reaction to touch of also less preferred grooming sites, an additional line for touching around the ears was added in TTEAM. Because of that, the results of touch duration are difficult to compare between the two methods and only duration within each method is worth discussion. During T the duration was consistent, whereas throughout TTEAM the duration decreased, the latter showing the mare’s reception of touch was improved. This finding is further supported with higher touch scores (ρ = -0.55; P<0.001) being associated with decreased touch duration (ρ = -0.55; P<0.001).
Leading protocol
A significant difference was found in the leading scores between mares (F = 22.46; η
2 = 0.67; P<0.001). The scores ranged from 0 to 4.7 (Fig 3). Scores significantly differed between testing days (F = 10.74; η
2 = 0.34; P<0.001). During the first six days, the lowest score of 2.2±1.2 was recorded on the 1st day and the highest score of 4.5±1.5 on the 5th day. Up to the 5
th day there was a gradual increase in the score. There was no change in score when the method was changed and up to 12
th day the score was constant with no statistical differences. There was a significantly higher leading score in the TTEAM compared to T (Z = -3.47; P = 0.001; Fig 4). During leading, it appears that the absence of forced cooperation during TTEAM accounted for higher scoring compared to T. Positive reinforcements were proposed to induce positive attitude towards humans and better long-term horse-human interaction
(Sankey et al., 2010). In contrast, the use of negative reinforcements may lead to stress and increased occurrence of agonistic behaviors, including aggression (
McGreevy and McLean 2005). Considering correlations, higher leading scores were associated with lower leading duration (ρ = -0.33; P<0.001).
There was no significant difference in the leading duration between mares (F = 1.48, η
2 = 0.13; P = 0.10). Duration significantly differed between testing days (F = 3.43, η
2 = 0.16; P<0.0001). From 1
st to 6
th day the duration gradually decreased, with the longest time of leading being 98.9±94.4 s on the 2
nd day and the lowest of 51.9±51.8 s (Fig 4) on the 5th day (P<0.001). Duration increased when the method was changed (P<0.001) but was then constant until the 12
th day. There was no difference in duration between the methods (t = 1.57, P = 0.12; Fig 4). This is surprising knowing that during TTEAM mares were free to walk at their own pace and explore the environment and one would expect that it would be more time consuming. This found indifference, but higher leading scores and significant association between higher leading scores and decreased leading duration, TTEAM appears to be useful method for increasing horses’ cooperation, reducing agonistic behaviors, while not being time consuming at the same time. The positive effects of TTEAM on leading were also reported by
Shanahan (2003), who found training with TTEAM to help decrease loading time, saliva cortisol concentration and heart rate during trailer loading.
Although, on average, mares showed more positive behavior during TTEAM in the majority of the observed parameters, there were significant individual differences found within (intra-individual variability) and between mares (inter-individual variability). Intra-specific variation, calculated as CRP, ranged between 0.02 and 2.95. CRP differed between parameters and was 0.34±0.51 for touch score, the highest for leading score (0.92±0.61), 0.64±0.96 for touch duration and the lowest for leading duration (0.34±0.30). By finding intra-individual variability in behavioral plasticity, we highlighted the importance of horse’s individuality, regardless of the handling method. This may not be surprising knowing that the experience of tactile interaction
(Ahrendt et al., 2015) and response to handling
(Hartmann et al., 2021) varies among individual horses. For this reason, not only handling method per se, but also other factors affecting handling responses should be taken into account when choosing the appropriate handling method for an individual horse.
For consideration in future research, we believe it is important to address certain limitations of our crossover study. Having a small sample size, we did not have enough power to generate scientifically valid and reproducible data if observing two separate groups of mares, each being tested with one handling method. Given the testing order, it could be argued that our mares could get habituated to humans, testing environments and procedures. However, it seems less likely this to happen, since habituation in our mares would translate into a decrease of negative behavioral responses, resulting in exponentially increased or constant scores. Such scoring was not observed in our study, implying the changes in behavior were not influenced by habituation to humans.